Why Bilinguals Are Smarter
Harriet Russell
By YUDHIJIT BHATTACHARJEE
SPEAKING two languages rather than just one has obvious practical
benefits in an increasingly globalized world. But in recent years,
scientists have begun to show that the advantages of bilingualism are
even more fundamental than being able to converse with a wider range of
people. Being bilingual, it turns out, makes you smarter. It can have a
profound effect on your brain, improving cognitive skills not related to
language and even shielding against dementia in old age.
This view of bilingualism is remarkably different from the understanding
of bilingualism through much of the 20th century. Researchers,
educators and policy makers long considered a second language to be an
interference, cognitively speaking, that hindered a child’s academic and
intellectual development.
They were not wrong about the interference: there is ample evidence that
in a bilingual’s brain both language systems are active even when he is
using only one language, thus creating situations in which one system
obstructs the other. But this interference, researchers are finding out,
isn’t so much a handicap as a blessing in disguise. It forces the brain
to resolve internal conflict, giving the mind a workout that
strengthens its cognitive muscles.
Bilinguals, for instance, seem to be more adept than monolinguals at solving certain kinds of mental puzzles. In a 2004 study
by the psychologists Ellen Bialystok and Michelle Martin-Rhee,
bilingual and monolingual preschoolers were asked to sort blue circles
and red squares presented on a computer screen into two digital bins —
one marked with a blue square and the other marked with a red circle.
In the first task, the children had to sort the shapes by color, placing
blue circles in the bin marked with the blue square and red squares in
the bin marked with the red circle. Both groups did this with comparable
ease. Next, the children were asked to sort by shape, which was more
challenging because it required placing the images in a bin marked with a
conflicting color. The bilinguals were quicker at performing this task.
The collective evidence from a number of such studies suggests that the
bilingual experience improves the brain’s so-called executive function —
a command system that directs the attention processes that we use for
planning, solving problems and performing various other mentally
demanding tasks. These processes include ignoring distractions to stay
focused, switching attention willfully from one thing to another and
holding information in mind — like remembering a sequence of directions
while driving.
Why does the tussle between two simultaneously active language systems
improve these aspects of cognition? Until recently, researchers thought
the bilingual advantage stemmed primarily from an ability for inhibition that
was honed by the exercise of suppressing one language system: this
suppression, it was thought, would help train the bilingual mind to
ignore distractions in other contexts. But that explanation increasingly
appears to be inadequate, since studies have shown that bilinguals
perform better than monolinguals even at tasks that do not require
inhibition, like threading a line through an ascending series of numbers
scattered randomly on a page.
The key difference between bilinguals and monolinguals may be more
basic: a heightened ability to monitor the environment. “Bilinguals have
to switch languages quite often — you may talk to your father in one
language and to your mother in another language,” says Albert Costa, a
researcher at the University of Pompeu Fabra in Spain. “It requires
keeping track of changes around you in the same way that we monitor our
surroundings when driving.” In a study comparing German-Italian
bilinguals with Italian monolinguals on monitoring tasks, Mr. Costa and
his colleagues found that the bilingual subjects not only performed
better, but they also did so with less activity in parts of the brain
involved in monitoring, indicating that they were more efficient at it.
The bilingual experience appears to influence the brain from infancy to
old age (and there is reason to believe that it may also apply to those
who learn a second language later in life).
In a 2009 study led by Agnes Kovacs
of the International School for Advanced Studies in Trieste, Italy,
7-month-old babies exposed to two languages from birth were compared
with peers raised with one language. In an initial set of trials, the
infants were presented with an audio cue and then shown a puppet on one
side of a screen. Both infant groups learned to look at that side of the
screen in anticipation of the puppet. But in a later set of trials,
when the puppet began appearing on the opposite side of the screen, the
babies exposed to a bilingual environment quickly learned to switch
their anticipatory gaze in the new direction while the other babies did
not.
Bilingualism’s effects also extend into the twilight years. In a recent
study of 44 elderly Spanish-English bilinguals, scientists led by the
neuropsychologist Tamar Gollan of the University of California, San
Diego, found that individuals with a higher degree of bilingualism —
measured through a comparative evaluation of proficiency in each
language — were more resistant than others to the onset of dementia and
other symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease: the higher the degree of
bilingualism, the later the age of onset.
Nobody ever doubted the power of language. But who would have imagined
that the words we hear and the sentences we speak might be leaving such a
deep imprint?
1 comment:
This is so interesting Nekky! I have no doubt that this is a fact . I studied Spanish in school and I am sorry that I did not master it. I am sure I bookmarked a site that teaches Spanish. I better get to it and sharpen my brain!:)
Post a Comment